新月直播

By Dick Anderson Photos by Marc Campos
Professor of Psychology Nancy Dess

From Summer 2009: Professor of Psychology Nancy Dess measures emotionality and taste through her research with rats. How did one fincky rodent send her studies in a new direction?

Editor's note: Psychology Professor Nancy Dess will retire this spring after 36 years at 新月直播. Dess and her work were profiled in the Summer 2009 翱肠肠颈诲别苍迟补濒听magazine.

Nancy Dess鈥檚 rats do not have names鈥攖hey are laboratory animals, not pets鈥攂ut that makes it a little difficult to write a story about the one rodent in particular who changed the course of her research into taste and emotions at Occidental. So, with apologies to Dess鈥攁 professor and chair of the psychology department and the 2008 winner of the Graham L. Sterling Memorial Award for teaching, service, and distinguished professional achievement鈥攍et鈥檚 call this exceptional rat 鈥淢ilo.鈥

But before we get to Milo鈥檚 story, a little background is in order. Like humans, rats have a fear of novelty鈥攁 condition known as neophobia. Stress and anxiety can increase neophobia, and saccharin (the high-potency sweetener in Sweet鈥檔 Low) is considered more novel to the palate than other sweeteners. 鈥淚t鈥檚 a funny taste,鈥 says Dess, who began to research taste emotionality and the organization of behavior late in her graduate studies and early in my postdoctoral careers. 鈥淚t has a funky, metallic, or bitter aftertaste.鈥

As part of an experiment, Dess arranged to have a group of rats be predisposed to a saccharin solution before receiving a physical stressor in the form of a mild electric shock, thus taking away the novelty of the taste.

But a funny thing happened on the way to the stressor. 鈥淲e gave a bunch of rats the saccharin to drink,鈥濃圖ess recalls, 鈥渁nd sure enough, we came in the next day, and they鈥檇 all drunk a nice healthy amount鈥攅xcept for this one rat who had drunk none of it.鈥 That would be the rodent hereafter known as Milo.

After checking on Milo鈥檚 health (鈥淗e was fine鈥擨 looked in, and he looked back at me鈥) and tapping the spout of the stainless steel metal water bottle for air bubbles, she returned the following day to find the same results. She switched the bottles with the plain water and saccharin-sweetened bottle; Milo switched sides as well. 鈥淭his was a rat,鈥 she says, 鈥渨ho did not like saccharin.鈥

Most rats, if they are given any sweetened solution, will drink it more avidly than if you just give them water. And the mystery of Milo 鈥渂rought us to the fork in the road,鈥 Dess continues. 鈥淭he most common and ex颅perimental response to this would be to throw him out; he鈥檚 a weirdo.鈥濃圔ut because Milo seemed really interesting鈥攁nd because she says 新月直播 has a history of encouraging faculty 鈥渢o be active scholars and to do interesting work鈥濃攖he decision was made to follow him.

They started selective breeding, taking Milo and a second rat 鈥渨ho was not nearly as remarkable鈥濃攁nd thus will remain nameless鈥斺渂ut drank less than an average amount of saccharin,鈥 and mating the two of them with females that measured a bit low on the saccharin-consuming side of things. Conversely, they took their highest saccharin-consuming rats and bred them together.

That was 33 generations ago. 鈥淲hat we now have are Occidental high- and low-saccharin-consuming rats,鈥濃圖ess says of the white albino rodents with the beady pink eyes. 鈥淵ou really can鈥檛 tell these rats apart by looking at them. You have to look at their behavior to see the evidence of this selective breeding process.鈥

鈥淥ne of the things I love so much about science is the mix of the orderly systematic planful part of it with the getting thrown for a loop serendipitous something that you never imagined happens,鈥 she says.

In the worldview of Nancy Dess, 鈥淪ugar is not an arbitrary commodity. The human shape of the world, the economic systems, the political systems, the trade routes related to those, the cultural glue and tensions that are related to commodity transfer鈥攖hat might all look very different if we didn鈥檛 like sugar so much,鈥 she said at the conclusion of her Sterling Award Lecture at 新月直播 on April 20.

The birth and maturation cycle of a rat is a speedy one: 21 days' gestation, and 21 days to weaning. At three weeks, 新月直播's low- and high-saccharin rats are divided by sex and placed in living quarters with their siblings until they are assigned to an experiment.
鈥淢y guess is a lot of people in charge at critical points in history have been more like high-saccharin rats,鈥 she added. 鈥淚t鈥檚 not just that they like sugar. They鈥檙e more impulsive; they鈥檙e less vulnerable to stress; they鈥檙e less anxious. Those are really good traits for putting brakes on unpleasant social things.鈥

Dess first developed an interest in psychology after taking an introductory course as an undergraduate at UCLA. From the beginning, what interested her most were the day-to-day questions of how things work鈥攚hat she calls 鈥渢he mystery in the ordinary.鈥

Despite the fact that her older brother had majored in psychology at UCLA (he鈥檚 now a clinical psychologist in San Diego), 鈥淚 was never that interested in a clinical or counseling career,鈥 notes Dess, who got her feet wet doing lab research as junior.

鈥淎 graduate student came to my physiological psychology class of 350 and said that he was looking for a couple of volunteers to help him with his dissertation research,鈥 she recalls. 鈥淪o I volunteered, and it was a rat lab that was looking at basic biological mechanisms related to Parkinson鈥檚. I loved working with the rats, but I was not that interested in the more mechanistic neuroscience side of things. I was more interested in their behavior and what they were doing.鈥

When it came time to choose a graduate program, Dess opted for one that emphasized behavioral analysis. That led her to the University of Minnesota, where she studied under 鈥渁n amazing mentor,鈥 Bruce Over颅mier, who is not only active in U.S. psychology

circles but also has been president of the International Union for Psychological Sciences. 鈥淚 can see now that his influence showed me how can you focus on your research and think about it in a bigger context,鈥 she says.

With Overmier鈥檚 encouragement, Dess became active in the American Psychological Association. She is currently the president of APA鈥檚 Division 6, the behavioral science and comparative psychology division, and has been a senior scientist at the association as well as chairing the APA鈥檚 Committee on Animal Research and Ethics. 鈥淭hat reflects Bruce鈥檚 influence, which says the world is not just your playground鈥攜ou have some disciplinary stewardship looking after the health and vitality and the integrity of the enterprise that you are a part of,鈥 Dess says.

Over the years, studies have shown that high-saccharin rats consume more of anything good that you give them. On the flip side, low-saccharin rats are more sensitive to the adulteration of something sweet with something bitter.

And what Dess and her colleagues have been doing over the last 15 years is pursuing three lines of inquiry: Are these individual differences in saccharin level related in anyway to the animal鈥檚 emotionality, including stress? (In a word, yes.) Second, do these high- and low-saccharin rats go about life provisioning themselves differently, and responding differently to metabolic threats? (鈥淲e, of course, would expect that they would.鈥) And lastly, does their willingness to consume saccharin in large amounts without restraint suggest a penchant for drug abuse?

Among their findings:鈥圵hen you sharply limit its food supply to one hour a day, a rat experiences depression-induced hyperacti颅vity鈥攚hich, in the case of the low-saccharin rat, averages about to about 11,000 running-wheel revolutions (or about seven miles) over a 24-hour period. 鈥淭hat鈥檚 a long way for a little animal to go,鈥濃圖ess says.

Another outcome is that 鈥淗igh-saccharin animals learn to self administer cocaine much more rapidly than low saccharin animals,鈥 she adds. 鈥淚t could be they鈥檙e just smarter, or it could be they鈥檙e more sensitive to any drug with the potential for abuse.鈥

Having published five papers just on taste, six on subjects other than taste, and six more on the psychopharmacology involved, Dess averages roughly a paper a year on her rat research alone. (She has authored more than three dozen peer-review articles, nine of which have 新月直播 students as co-authors.)

鈥淥ne of the bum raps that science sometimes gets is that it鈥檚 really all confirmatory,鈥 she says. While noting that the 鈥渉it rate鈥 of 新月直播鈥檚 research is pretty good, 鈥淲e鈥檝e had some big wrong predictions that we were pretty certain of that turned into really interesting things. 鈥 I also have benefited greatly in terms of balancing the risk with the certainty from being part of collaborations with people at other institutions.鈥

She points to the work that Marilyn Carroll, a professor of psychiatry at the University of Minnesota, was doing with regards to impulsivity and the behavioral characteristics related to drug use. 鈥淪he was all set up to do drug administration,鈥濃圖ess says, 鈥渟o all we had to do is send her some rats.鈥

Marking the rodents鈥 tails鈥攂lue for high-saccharin, and red for low-saccharin鈥擠ess shipped live rats to Minnesota about seven years ago. Carroll subsequently bred them, and now the university has the only other colony of low- and high-saccharin rats in existence. 鈥淗er graduate students and postdocs have been publishing really interesting studies,鈥 says Dess.

Typically, there are around 200 rats living in laboratory housing at 新月直播, in a former storage facility and recreational room converted some years ago by professor of psychology emeritus Dennis VanderWeele. After a study is completed, the rats who participated are given saccharin-intake tests, and the highest drinkers of the highs and lowest drinkers of the lows are selected for breeding. 鈥淏reeding is a treadmill,鈥濃圖ess says. 鈥淲e鈥檝e always got more in the pipeline.鈥

Thirty-three generations removed from Milo, is there an end to the high- and low-saccharin rat studies in sight? 鈥淚 try to never say never, but I try to never say always,鈥 Dess says. 鈥淭here are still things we want to know. Right now we have one tantalizing and frustrating puzzle that we鈥檙e working on鈥濃攁n experiment studying sugar addiction in rats that springs out of a series of studies conducted by Veronica Yakenveno 鈥09, a psychology major from Odessa, Ukraine鈥斺渁nd we have another dreamlike series of experiments that we鈥檙e following up where every experiment comes out cleanly and straight-forwardly鈥濃攁 study on flavor-preference conditioning that may explain what makes people with a taste for alcohol drink more.

What is it about rats that makes them ideal for laboratory research? 鈥淚 think there are multiple reasons, and practicality is a big one,鈥濃圖ess says. 鈥淭hey are small, they are rapidly reproducing, they are hearty, and they have large litters. But there are other animals with some of those attributes that never quite caught on, like gerbils and hamsters.鈥 One key distinction, she adds, is that rats are a domesticated species鈥攈amsters and gerbils aren鈥檛.

鈥淚t鈥檚 a pretty small percentage of people who are working with nonhuman animals at all,鈥濃圖ess says. 鈥淭he preferred species in psychology is human.鈥